2.24.2014

Ted at TED

Ted Miguel gives a TED talk explaining our work on climate and conflict.



I've been waiting a month to use that title.

2.21.2014

USF's IDEC Masters Program Now Recruiting


The University of San Francisco's Masters program in International and Development Economics is currently accepting applicants through early March. The program is fairly unique among econ masters programs for having 3 semesters of econometrics (I teach the first class in the sequence), and a mandatory field work portion during the summer between years one and two. It also offers a mix of elective classes in applied micro and international / macro that one would normally be hard pressed to find at the Master's level, including Alessandra Cassar's Experimental Economics class and my survey course in Environment and Development Economics. Graduates complete an original thesis under the advisory oversight of a faculty member, and recent topics have spanned the gamut from in-utero impacts of rainfall shocks in Bangladesh to determinants of gender-differentiated competition in China. If you or someone you know is interested in pursuing graduate education in development and has a bent towards quantitative methods, drop me an email and we can chat.

2.17.2014

Ambient carbon dioxide affects human decision-making

Amir Jina and I recently visited William Fisk at LBL who pointed us to his fascinating study:


Usha Satish, Mark J. Mendell, Krishnamurthy Shekhar, Toshifumi Hotchi, Douglas Sullivan, Siegfried Streufert, and William J. Fisk

Abstract:
Background: Associations of higher indoor carbon dioxide (CO2) concentrations with impaired work performance, increased health symptoms, and poorer perceived air quality have been attributed to correlation of indoor CO2 with concentrations of other indoor air pollutants that are also influenced by rates of outdoor-air ventilation. 
Objectives: We assessed direct effects of increased CO2, within the range of indoor concentrations, on decision making. 
Methods: Twenty-two participants were exposed to CO2 at 600, 1,000, and 2,500 ppm in an office-like chamber, in six groups. Each group was exposed to these conditions in three 2.5-hr sessions, all on 1 day, with exposure order balanced across groups. At 600 ppm, CO2 came from outdoor air and participants’ respiration. Higher concentrations were achieved by injecting ultrapure CO2. Ventilation rate and temperature were constant. Under each condition, participants completed a computer-based test of decision-making performance as well as questionnaires on health symptoms and perceived air quality. Participants and the person administering the decision-making test were blinded to CO2 level. Data were analyzed with analysis of variance models. 
Results: Relative to 600 ppm, at 1,000 ppm CO2, moderate and statistically significant decrements occurred in six of nine scales of decision-making performance. At 2,500 ppm, large and statistically significant reductions occurred in seven scales of decision-making performance (raw score ratios, 0.06–0.56), but performance on the focused activity scale increased. 
Conclusions: Direct adverse effects of CO2 on human performance may be economically important and may limit energy-saving reductions in outdoor air ventilation per person in buildings. Confirmation of these findings is needed.

From the LBL press page
On nine scales of decision-making performance, test subjects showed significant reductions on six of the scales at CO2 levels of 1,000 parts per million (ppm) and large reductions on seven of the scales at 2,500 ppm. The most dramatic declines in performance, in which subjects were rated as “dysfunctional,” were for taking initiative and thinking strategically. “Previous studies have looked at 10,000 ppm, 20,000 ppm; that’s the level at which scientists thought effects started,” said Berkeley Lab scientist Mark Mendell, also a co-author of the study. “That’s why these findings are so startling.” 
The primary source of indoor CO2 is humans. While typical outdoor concentrations are around 380 ppm, indoor concentrations can go up to several thousand ppm. Higher indoor CO2 concentrations relative to outdoors are due to low rates of ventilation, which are often driven by the need to reduce energy consumption. In the real world, CO2 concentrations in office buildings normally don’t exceed 1,000 ppm, except in meeting rooms, when groups of people gather for extended periods of time. 
In classrooms, concentrations frequently exceed 1,000 ppm and occasionally exceed 3,000 ppm. CO2 at these levels has been assumed to indicate poor ventilation, with increased exposure to other indoor pollutants of potential concern, but the CO2 itself at these levels has not been a source of concern. Federal guidelines set a maximum occupational exposure limit at 5,000 ppm as a time-weighted average for an eight-hour workday.

2.12.2014

Spatial Data and Analysis

I developed a new course last fall title "Spatial Data and Analysis". Because several people have asked for the material, I've finally posted the syllabus and assignments online here

Description of the course:
The recent explosion of spatially explicit data and analytical tools, such as "Geographic Information Systems" (GIS) and spatial econometrics, have aided researchers and decision- makers faced with a variety of challenges. This course introduces students to spatial data and its analysis, as well as the modeling of spatially dependent social processes and policy problems. Students will be introduced to the types, sources, and display of spatial data. Through hands-on analysis, students will learn to extract quantitative information from spatial data for applied research and public policy. Students will be introduced to spatial statistics, spatially dependent simulation, and spatial optimization. Students will learn to think creatively about spatial problems through examples drawn from economics, politics, epidemiology, criminology, agriculture, social networks, and the environment. The goal of the course is to equip advanced masters students and doctoral students with tools that will help them be effective analysts and communicators of spatial information in their future research or policy-related work. Because hands-on analysis plays a central role in the class, students will benefit from prior experience with basic computer programming -- although prior experience is not required. Prerequisites: introductory statistics or equivalent.

1.17.2014

FAQs for "Reconciling disagreement over climate–conflict results in Africa"

[This is a gues blog post by my coauthor Kyle Meng.]

Sol and I just published an article in PNAS in which we reexamine a controversy in the climate-conflict literature. The debate is centered over two previous PNAS articles: the first by Burke et al. (PNAS, 2009) which claims that higher temperature increases conflict risks in sub-Saharan Africa and a second PNAS article by Buhaug (PNAS, 2010) refuting the earlier study.

How did we get here?

First, a bit of background. Whether climate change causes societies to be more violent is a critical question for our understanding of climate impacts. If climate change indeed increases violence, the economic and social costs of climate change may be far greater than what was previously considered, and thus further prompt the need to reduce greenhouse gas emissions. To answer this question, researchers in recent years have turned to data from the past asking whether violence has responded historically to changes in the local climate. Despite the increasing volume of research (summarized by Sol, Marshall Burke, and Ted Miguel in their meta-analysis published in Science and the accompanying review article in Climatic Change) this question remained somewhat controversial in the public eye. Much of this controversy was generated by this pair of PNAS papers.

What did we do?

Our new paper takes a fresh look at these two prior studies by statistically examining whether the evidence provided by Buhaug (2010) overturns the results in Burke et al. (2009). Throughout, we examine the two central claims made by Buhaug:
1) that Burke et al.'s results "do not hold up to closer inspection" and
2) climate change does not cause conflict in sub-Saharan Africa.  
Because these are quantitative papers, Buhaug’s two claims can be answered using statistical methods. What we found was that Buhaug did not run the appropriate statistical procedures needed for the claims made. When we applied the correct statistical tests, we find that:
a) the evidence in Buhaug is not statistically different from that of Burke et al. and
b) Buhaug’s results cannot support the claim that climate does not cause conflict. 
A useful analogy

The statistical reasoning in our paper is a bit technical so an analogy may be helpful here. Burke et al's main result is equivalent to saying "smoking increases lung cancer risks roughly 10%". Buhaug claims above are equivalent to stating that his analysis demonstrates that “smoking does not increase lung cancer risks” and furthermore that “smoking does not affect lung cancer risks at all”.

What we find, after applying the appropriate statistical method, is that the only equivalent claim that can be supported by Buhaug’s analysis is "smoking may increase lung cancer risks by roughly 100% or may decrease them by roughly 100% or may have no effect whatsoever". Notice this is a far different statement than what Buhaug claims he has demonstrated in 1) and 2) above. Basically, the results presented in Buhaug are so uncertain that they do not reject zero effect, but they also do not reject the original work by Burke et al.

Isn’t Buhaug just showing Burke et al.’s result is “not robust”?

In statistical analyses, we often seek to understand if a result is “robust” by demonstrating that reasonable alterations to the model do not produce dramatically different results. If successful, this type of analysis sometimes convinces us that we have not failed to account for important omitted variables (or other factors) that would alter our estimates substantively.

Importantly, however, the reverse logic is not true and “non-robustness” is not a conclusive (or logical) result. Obtaining different estimates from the application of model alterations alone does not necessarily imply that the original result is wrong since it might be the new estimate that is biased.   Observing unstable results suggests that there are errors in the specification of some (or all) of the models.  It merely means the analyst isn’t working with the right statistical model.

There must exist only one  “true” relationship between climate and conflict, it may be a coefficient of zero or a larger coefficient consistent with Burke et al., but it cannot be all these coefficients at the same time. If models with very different underlying assumptions provide dramatically different estimates, this suggests that all of the models (except perhaps one) is misspecified and should be thrown out.

A central error in Buhaug is his interpretation of his findings.  He removes critical parts of Burke et al.’s model (e.g. those that account for important differences in geography, history and culture) or re-specifies them in other ways and then advocates that the various inconsistent coefficients produced should all be taken seriously. In reality, the varying estimates produced by Buhaug are either due to added model biases or to sampling uncertainty caused by the techniques that he is using. It is incorrect to interpret this variation as evidence that Burke et al.’s estimate is “non-robust”.

So are you saying Burke et al. was right?

No. And this is a very important point. In our article, we carefully state:
“It is important to note that our findings neither confirm nor reject the results of Burke et al.. Our results simply reconcile the apparent contradiction between Burke et al. and Buhaug by demonstrating that Buhaug does not provide evidence that contradicts the results reported in Burke et al. Notably, however, other recent analyses obtain results that largely agree with Burke et al., so we think it is likely that analyses following our approach will reconcile any apparent disagreement between these other studies and Buhaug.”
That is, taking Burke et al’s result as given, we find that the evidence provided in Buhaug does not refute Burke et al. (the central claim of Buhaug). Whether Burke et al. was right about climate causing conflict in sub-Saharan Africa is a different question. We’ve tried to answer that question in other settings (e.g. our joint work published in Nature), but that’s not the contribution of this analysis.

Parting note

Lastly, we urge those interested to read our article carefully. Simply skimming the paper by hunting for statistically significant results would be missing the paper’s point. Our broader hope besides helping to reconcile this prior controversy is that the statistical reasoning underlying our work becomes more common in data-driven analyses.

1.16.2014

Fourth Interdisciplinary Ph.D. Workshop in Sustainable Development

The students of the Columbia Sustainable Development Ph.D. program have put out the call for papers for the Fourth Interdisciplinary Ph.D. Workshop in Sustainable Development. It's a great opportunity for Ph.D. students to meet colleagues from a broad array of disciplines, and a bunch of our younger colleagues will be there. Please pass it along.

Fourth Interdisciplinary Ph.D. Workshop in Sustainable Development
April 25th-26th, 2014: Columbia University in the City of New York, USA

The graduate students in the Sustainable Development PhD program at Columbia University are convening the Fourth Interdisciplinary Ph.D. Workshop in Sustainable Development (IPWSD); scheduled for April 25th-26th, 2014, at Columbia University in New York City.

The IPWSD is a conference open to graduate students working on or interested in issues related to sustainable development.  It is intended to provide a forum to present and discuss research in an informal setting, as well as to meet and interact with similar graduate student researchers from other institutions.  In particular, we hope to facilitate a network among students pursuing in-depth research across a range of disciplines in the social and natural sciences, to generate a larger interdisciplinary discussion concerning sustainable development.  If your research pertains to the field of sustainable development and the linkages between natural and social systems, we encourage you to apply regardless of disciplinary background.

For details, please see the call for papers, or visit our conference website where a detailed list of topics, conference themes and other information is available.

Please share this information widely with graduate students and other interested parties. We look forward to seeing you in New York City in April!

With kind regards,

The Fourth IPWSD Planning Committee
Sustainable Development Doctoral Society, 
Columbia University
Contact: cu.sdds.ipwsd@gmail.com

1.15.2014

Reconciling disagreement over climate–conflict results in Africa

Kyle and I have a paper out in the Early Edition of PNAS this week:

Reconciling disagreement over climate–conflict results in Africa
Solomon M. Hsiang and Kyle C. Meng
Abstract: A recent study by Burke et al. [Burke M, Miguel E, Satyanath S, Dykema J, Lobell D (2009) Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106(49):20670– 20674] reports statistical evidence that the likelihood of civil wars in African countries was elevated in hotter years. A following study by Buhaug [Buhaug H (2010) Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107 (38):16477–16482] reports that a reexamination of the evidence overturns Burke et al.’s findings when alternative statistical models and alternative measures of conflict are used. We show that the conclusion by Buhaug is based on absent or incorrect statistical tests, both in model selection and in the comparison of results with Burke et al. When we implement the correct tests, we find there is no evidence presented in Buhaug that rejects the original results of Burke et al. 
Related reconciliation of different results in Kenya.

A brief refresher and discussion of the controversy that we are examining is here.

1.13.2014

Climate-conflict research, before the IRB...

Finding old papers on temperature manipulation is turning into a hobby of mine. I actually had to go to the library to dig up this gem. From Rohles, Frederick H. "Environmental psychology: A bucket of worms." Psychology Today 1.2 (1967): 55-63.



1.10.2014

Reconciling temperature-conflict results in Kenya

Marshall, Ted and I have a new short working paper out. When we correct the coding of a single variable in a previous study (that uses a new data set), we obtain highly localized temperature-conflict associations in Kenya that are largely in line with the rest of the literature. I think this is a useful example for why we should be careful with how we specify interaction terms.

Reconciling temperature-conflict results in Kenya
Solomon M. Hsiang, Marshall Burke, and Edward Miguel
Abstract: Theisen (JPR, 2012) recently constructed a novel high-resolution data set of intergroup and political conflict in Kenya (1989-2004) and examined whether the risk of conflict onset and incidence responds to annual pixel-level variations in temperature and precipitation.  Thiesen concluded that only extreme precipitation is associated with conflict incidence and that temperature is unrelated to conflict, seemingly at odds with recent studies that found a positive association at the pixel scale (O'laughlin et al., PNAS 2012), at the country scale (Burke et al., PNAS 2009), and at the continental scale (Hsiang et al., Nature 2011) in Africa.  Here we show these findings can be reconciled when we correct the erroneous coding of temperature-squared in Thiesen. In contrast to the original conclusions presented in Theisen, both conflict onset and conflict incidence are significantly and positively associated with local temperature in this new and independently assembled data set.

12.17.2013

Landsat at your fingertips

The USGS has put together a slick GUI that let's you browse (as if it were GoogleEarth) and download Landsat data. The interface is described here.

One of my students found this and showed it to me. (Over the next few weeks, hopefully I'll be able to post much of the material and discoveries from my new course "Spatial Data and Analysis".)



12.09.2013

What is identification?

There are relatively few non-academic internet resources on identification and causal inference in the social sciences, especially of the sort that can be consumed by a nonspecialist. To remedy that slightly I decided to tidy up and post some slides I've used to give talks on causal inference a few times in the past year. They're aimed at senior undergrad or graduate students with at least some background in statistics or econometrics, and can be found here:

Causal Inference, Identification, and Identification Strategies

Feel free to drop me a line and give me feedback, especially if somethings seems unclear / incorrect. Thanks!

11.18.2013

Year of Reviews in Review: The New Environment and Development Literature

Amir Jina and I were recently discussing the multiple literature reviews that have come out on environment and development topics lately, and realized that there were so many we were starting to lose track. To that end, and as a service to those of you who aren't constantly trawling the working paper and journal lists, here's a quick rundown of the over ten (and counting) recent literature reviews that have come out in the newly emerging environment and development literature:

11.13.2013

Destruction, Disinvestment, and Death: Economic and Human Losses Following Environmental Disaster

Typhoon Haiyan as seen from space, Copyright 2013 JMA/EUMETSAT
Last spring Sol and I finished up the working paper version of our paper "Destruction, Disinvestment, and Death: Economic and Human Losses Following Environmental Disaster." Since the paper is long and fairly technical, we decided it would be worthwhile to do a shorter, more general-audience-appropriate piece for the blog, something that seems especially relevant given Typhoon Haiyan's devastating landfall this past weekend. If you'd like to take a look at the paper itself, you can find a copy of it here on SSRN; a copy of the supplemental appendix can be found here.

The motivation for "Destruction, Disinvestment, and Death" stems from the fact that we actually know surprisingly little about how people fare in the wake of natural disasters.

10.21.2013

Climate, conflict, and social stability: what does the evidence say?

The "sister paper" to our recent Science article on climate and conflict has come out in Climatic Change. This new article is a traditional review article that walks readers through individual studies in the literature and discusses some of the debates in less technical terms than the Science article. The sample of studies included is slightly different since the submission timeline for the two studies was different.

Climate, conflict, and social stability: what does the evidence say?
Solomon Hsiang and Marshall Burke
Abstract: Are violent conflict and socio-political stability associated with changes in climatological variables? We examine 50 rigorous quantitative studies on this question and find consistent support for a causal association between climatological changes and various conflict outcomes, at spatial scales ranging from individual buildings to the entire globe and at temporal scales ranging from an anomalous hour to an anomalous millennium. Multiple mechanisms that could explain this association have been proposed and are sometimes supported by findings, but the literature is currently unable to decisively exclude any proposed pathway. Several mechanisms likely contribute to the outcomes that we observe.

9.20.2013

Envirodevonomics

There's a new working paper by Michael Greenstone and Kelsey Jack that's of obvious interest to FE readers:
Envirodevonomics: A Research Agenda for a Young Field
Environmental quality in many developing countries is poor and generates substantial health and productivity costs. However, existing measures of willingness to pay for environmental quality improvements indicate low valuations by affected households. This paper argues that this seeming paradox is the central puzzle at the intersection of environmental and development economics: Given poor environmental quality and high health burdens in developing countries, why is WTP so low? We develop a conceptual framework for understanding this puzzle and propose four potential explanations: (1) due to low income levels, individuals value increases in income more than marginal improvements in environmental quality, (2) the marginal costs of environmental quality improvements are high, (3) political economy factors undermine efficient policy-making, and (4) market failures such as weak property rights and missing capital markets drive a wedge between true and revealed willingness to pay for environmental quality. We review the available literature on each explanation and discuss how the framework also applies to climate change, which is perhaps the most important issue at the intersection of environment and development economics. The paper concludes with a list of promising and unanswered research questions for the emerging sub-field of “envirodevonomics.”

9.18.2013

New GIS Data for the Demographic and Health Surveys

USAID's Measure Demographic and Health Surveys (DHS) are an extraordinary (free) data set on maternal and child health from around the world. They've just released their new spatial data repository which, among other things, adds crucial shape files of subnational region borders over time. From the official announcement:
We are pleased to announce the launch of a new open data GIS resource from MEASURE DHS.                                                                          
The Spatial Data Repository provides geographically-linked health and demographic data from the MEASURE Demographic and Health Surveys (DHS) project and the U.S. Census Bureau for mapping in a geographic information system (GIS).
-Boundaries of DHS regions can be explored to visualize change over time.
-Data from DHS indicators and U.S. Census population estimates can be downloaded in GIS format.
Please share with your colleagues and friends.
Go explore!  http://spatialdata.measuredhs.com/